Bibliography R-Z

Raethke, N. (2004). The role of olfaction during mating in the southern temperate spiny lobster Jasus edwardsii. Hormones and Behavior, 46(3), 311-318. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.04.005

Raethke, N., MacDiarmid, A. B., & Montgomery, J. C. (2004). The role of olfaction during mating in the southern temperate spiny lobster Jasus edwardsii. [Comparative Study Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Horm Behav, 46(3), 311-318. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.04.005

Ramm, S. A., & Stockley, P. (2009). Male house mice do not adjust sperm allocation in response to odours from related or unrelated rivals. Animal Behaviour, 78(3), 685-690. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.018

Range, F., Heucke, S. L., Gruber, C., Konz, A., Huber, L., & Virányi, Z. (2009). The effect of ostensive cues on dogs’ performance in a manipulative social learning task. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 120(3-4), 170-178. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.05.012

Rangel, J., & Seeley, T. (2008). The signals initiating the mass exodus of a honeybee swarm from its nest. Animal Behaviour, 76(6), 1943-1952. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.004

Rangel, J., & Seeley, T. D. (2008). The signals initiating the mass exodus of a honeybee swarm from its nest. Animal Behaviour, 76(6), 1943-1952. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.004

Reby, D., Charlton, B. D., Locatelli, Y., & McComb, K. (2010). Oestrous red deer hinds prefer male roars with higher fundamental frequencies. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Proc Biol Sci, 277(1695), 2747-2753. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0467

Reby, D., McComb, K., Cargnelutti, B., Darwin, C., Fitch, W. T., & Clutton-Brock, T. (2005). Red deer stags use formants as assessment cues during intrasexual agonistic interactions. [Comparative Study Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Proc Biol Sci, 272(1566), 941-947. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2954

Remedios, R., Logothetis, N. K., & Kayser, C. (2009). Monkey drumming reveals common networks for perceiving vocal and nonvocal communication sounds. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 106(42), 18010-18015. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909756106

Rendall, D., Owren, M. J., & Ryan, M. J. (2009). What do animal signals mean? Animal Behaviour, 78(2), 233-240. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.007

Richards, D. B., & Thompson, N. S. (1978). Critical properties of the assembly call of the common American Crow. Behaviour, 64, 184-203.

Ries, C., Spaethe, J., Sztatecsny, M., Strondl, C., & Hödl, W. (2008). Turning blue and ultraviolet: sex-specific colour change during the mating season in the Balkan moor frog. Journal of Zoology, 276(3), 229-236. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2008.00456.x

Roberts, M. (1997). The Man Who Listens To Horses: The Story of a Real-Life Horse Whisperer. New York: Random House.

Robinson, G. E. (1996). Chemical communication in honeybees. Science, 271, 1824-1825.

Rooney, N. J., & Bradshaw, J. W. S. (2006). Social cognition in the domestic dog: behaviour of spectators towards participants in interspecific games. Animal Behaviour, 72(2), 343-352. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.014

Root-Bernstein, M. (2010). Displacement activities during the honeybee transition from waggle dance to foraging. Animal Behaviour, 79(4), 935-938. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.010

Rosenbaum, H. C., Pomilla, C., Mendez, M., Leslie, M. S., Best, P. B., Findlay, K. P., . . . Kiszka, J. (2009). Population structure of humpback whales from their breeding grounds in the South Atlantic and Indian Oceans. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. PLoS One, 4(10), e7318. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007318

Roush, R. S., & Snowdon, C. T. (2001). Food transfer and development of feeding behavior and food-associated vocalizations in Cotton-Top Tamarins. Ethology, 107, 415-429.

Rumbaugh, D. (1977). Language Learning by a Chimpanzee: The LANA Project. New York: Academic Press.

Rundus, A. S., Owings, D. H., Joshi, S. S., Chinn, E., & Giannini, N. (2007). Ground squirrels use an infrared signal to deter rattlesnake predation. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 104(36), 14372-14376. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702599104

Rusch, K. M., Pytte, C. L., & Ficken, M. S. (1996). Organization of agonistic vocalizations in black-chinned hummingbirds. The Condor, 98, 557-566.

Ryne, C. (2009). Homosexual interactions in bed bugs: alarm pheromones as male recognition signals. Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1471-1475. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.033

Ryne, C. (2009). Homosexual interactions in bed bugs: alarm pheromones as male recognition signals. Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1471-1475. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.033

Sakata, J. T., Hampton, C. M., & Brainard, M. S. (2008). Social modulation of sequence and syllable variability in adult birdsong. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. J Neurophysiol, 99(4), 1700-1711. doi: 10.1152/jn.01296.2007

Salazar, V. L., & Stoddard, P. K. (2009). Social competition affects electric signal plasticity and steroid levels in the gymnotiform fish Brachyhypopomus gauderio. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Horm Behav, 56(4), 399-409. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.07.009

Sánchez, D., Nieh, J. C., & Vandame, R. (2008). Experience-based interpretation of visual and chemical information at food sources in the stingless bee Scaptotrigona mexicana. Animal Behaviour, 76(2), 407-414. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.04.003

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1986). Ape Language: From Conditioned Response to Symbol. New York: Columbia University Press.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., & Lewin, R. (1994). Kanzi: An Ape at the Brink of Human Mind. New York: Wiley.

Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Fields, W. M., & Taglialatela, J. (2000). Ape consciousness-human consciousness: A perspective informed by language and culture. American Zoologist, 49, 910-921.

Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. G., & Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes, Language, and the Human Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Sayigh, L. S., Esch, H. C., Wells, R. S., & Janik, V. M. (2007). Facts about signature whistles of bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Animal Behaviour, 74(6), 1631-1642. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.02.018

Sayigh, L. S., Tyack, P. L., Wells, R. S., Solow, A. R., Scott, M. D., & Irvine, A. B. (1998). Individual recognition in wild bottlenose dolphins: A field test using playback experiments. Animal Behaviour, 57, 41-50.

Scarantino, A. (2010). Animal communication between information and influence. Animal Behaviour, 79(6), e1-e5. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.005

Scarantino, A. (2010). Animal communication between information and influence. Animal Behaviour, 79(6), e1-e5. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.005

Schel, A. M., Candiotti, A., & Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Predator-deterring alarm call sequences in Guereza colobus monkeys are meaningful to conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 80(5), 799-808. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.07.012

Schöner, C. R., Schöner, M. G., & Kerth, G. (2010). Similar is not the same: Social calls of conspecifics are more effective in attracting wild bats to day roosts than those of other bat species. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64(12), 2053-2063. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1019-8

Schulz, T. M., Whitehead, H., Gero, S., & Rendell, L. (2008). Overlapping and matching of codas in vocal interactions between sperm whales: insights into communication function. Animal Behaviour, 76(6), 1977-1988. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.032

Scott-Phillips, T. C. (2010). Animal communication: insights from linguistic pragmatics. Animal Behaviour, 79(1), e1-e4. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.10.013

Searby, A., & Jouventin, P. (2003). Mother-lamb acoustic recognition in sheep: a frequency coding. Proc Biol Sci, 270(1526), 1765-1771. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2442

Searcy, W. A., Anderson, R. C., & Nowicki, S. (2006). Bird song as a signal of aggressive intent. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 60(2), 234-241. doi: 10.1007/s00265-006-0161-9

Searcy, W. A., & Beecher, M. D. (2009). Song as an aggressive signal in songbirds. Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1281-1292. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.08.011

Searcy, W. A., & Nowicki, S. (2006). Signal Interception and the Use of Soft Song in Aggressive Interactions. Ethology, 112(9), 865-872. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01238.x

Sèbe, F., Duboscq, J., Aubin, T., Ligout, S., & Poindron, P. (2010). Early vocal recognition of mother by lambs: contribution of low- and high-frequency vocalizations. Animal Behaviour, 79(5), 1055-1066. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.021

Seeley, T. (1985). Honeybee Ecology: A Study of Adaptation in Social Life. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press.

Seeley, T. D. (1999). Born to dance. Natural History, 6/99, 54-57.

Seeley, T. D. (2010). Honeybee Democracy. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press.

Seeley, T. D., Kuhnholz, S., & Weidenmuller, A. (1996). The honey bee’s tremble dance stimulates additional bees to function as nectar receivers. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 39, 419-427.

Semple, S. (2001). Individuality and male discrimination of female copulation calls in the yellow baboon. Animal Behaviour, 61(5), 1023-1028. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1692

Senechal, S. (2009). Dogs Can Sign, Too: A Breakthrough Method for Teaching Your Dog to Communicate to You. Berkeley CA: Celestial Arts.

Sewall, K. B. (2009). Limited adult vocal learning maintains call dialects but permits pair-distinctive calls in red crossbills. Animal Behaviour, 77(5), 1303-1311. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.033

Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2003). Signalers and receivers in animal communication. [Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S. Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S. Review]. Annu Rev Psychol, 54, 145-173. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145121

Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., Bergman, T., Fischer, J., Zuberbühler, K., & Hammerschmidt, K. (2010). The central importance of information in studies of animal communication. Animal Behaviour, 80(1), 3-8. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.012

Shalter, M. D. (1978). Localization of passarine seet and mobbing calls by goshawks and pygmy owls. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 46, 260-267.

Shannon, C. E., & Weaver, W. (1949). The Mathematical Theory of Communication. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois.

Sharon, G., Segal, D., Ringo, J. M., Hefetz, A., Zilber-Rosenberg, I., & Rosenberg, E. (2010). Commensal bacteria play a role in mating preference of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 107(46), 20051-20056. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009906107

Shu, W., Cho, J. Y., Jiang, Y., Zhang, M., Weisz, D., Elder, G. A., . . . Buxbaum, J. D. (2005). Altered ultrasonic vocalization in mice with a disruption in the FoxP2 gene. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 102, 9643-9648.

Silk, J. B., Kaldor, E., & Boyd, R. (2000). Cheap talk when interests conflict. Anim Behav, 59(2), 423-432. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1312

Skelhorn, J., Griksaitis, D., & Rowe, C. (2008). Colour biases are more than a question of taste. Animal Behaviour, 75(3), 827-835. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.003

Slobodchikoff, C. N., Kiriazis, J., Fischer, C., & Creef, E. (1991). Semantic information distinguishing individual predators in the alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs. Animal Behaviour, 42, 713-719.

Slobodchikoff, C. N., Paseka, A., & Verdolin, J. L. (2009). Prairie dog alarm calls encode labels about predator colors. [Comparative Study]. Anim Cogn, 12(3), 435-439. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0203-y

Slobodchikoff, C. N., Perla, B. S., & Verdolin, J. L. (2009). Prairie Dogs: Communication and Community in an Animal Society. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

Slobodchikoff, C. N., & Placer, J. (2006). Acoustic structures in the alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(5), 3153-3160.

Slocombe, K. E., Kaller, T., Turman, L., Townsend, S. W., Papworth, S., Squibbs, P., & Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64(12), 1959-1966. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1006-0

Slocombe, K. E., Waller, B. M., & Liebal, K. (2011). The language void: the need for multimodality in primate communication research. Animal Behaviour, 81(5), 919-924. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.02.002

Slocombe, K. E., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2005). Agonistic screams in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) vary as a function of social role. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. J Comp Psychol, 119(1), 67-77. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.119.1.67

Smith, C. L., Van Dyk, D. A., Taylor, P. W., & Evans, C. S. (2009). On the function of an enigmatic ornament: wattles increase the conspicuousness of visual displays in male fowl. Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1433-1440. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.034

Snowdon, C. T., & Teie, D. (2010). Affective responses in tamarins elicited by species-specific music. [Comparative Study Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Biol Lett, 6(1), 30-32. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0593

Soard, C. M., & Ritchison, G. (2009). ‘Chick-a-dee’ calls of Carolina chickadees convey information about degree of threat posed by avian predators. Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1447-1453. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.026

Sober, S. J., & Brainard, M. S. (2009). Adult birdsong is actively maintained by error correction. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Nat Neurosci, 12(7), 927-931. doi: 10.1038/nn.2336

Soltis, J. (2009). What do animal signals do? Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1485-1486. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.030

Soltis, J., Leong, K., & Savage, A. (2005). African elephant vocal communication II: rumble variation reflects the individual identity and emotional state of callers. Animal Behaviour, 70(3), 589-599. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.11.016

Spillmann, B., Dunkel, L. P., van Noordwijk, M. A., Amda, R. N. A., Lameira, A. R., Wich, S. A., & van Schaik, C. P. (2010). Acoustic Properties of Long Calls Given by Flanged Male Orang-Utans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) Reflect Both Individual Identity and Context. Ethology, 116(5), 385-395. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01744.x

Stahler, D. (2002). Common ravens, Corvus corax, preferentially associate with grey wolves, Canis lupus, as a foraging strategy in winter. Animal Behaviour, 64(2), 283-290. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.3047

Stahler, D., Heinrich, B., & Smith, D. (2002). Common ravens, Corvus corax, preferentially associate with grey wolves, Canis lupus, as a foraging strategy in winter. Animal Behaviour, 64(2), 283-290. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.3047

Stephan, C., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2008). Predation increases acoustic complexity in primate alarm calls. [Comparative Study Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Biol Lett, 4(6), 641-644. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0488

Suzuki, R., Buck, J. R., & Tyack, P. L. (2006). Information entropy of humpback whale songs. J Acoust Soc Am, 119(3), 1849. doi: 10.1121/1.2161827

Suzuki, T. N. (2011). Parental alarm calls warn nestlings about different predatory threats. [Letter]. Curr Biol, 21(1), R15-16. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.027

Swan, D. C., & Hare, J. F. (2008). The first cut is the deepest: primary syllables of Richardson’s ground squirrel, Spermophilus richardsonii, repeated calls alert receivers. Animal Behaviour, 76(1), 47-54. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.11.008

Taglialatela, J. P., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., & Baker, L. A. (2003). Vocal production by a language-competent Pan paniscus. International Journal of Primatology, 24(1), 1-17.

Taylor, A. M., & Reby, D. (2010). The contribution of source-filter theory to mammal vocal communication research. Journal of Zoology, 280(3), 221-236. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2009.00661.x

Taylor, A. M., Reby, D., & McComb, K. (2008). Human listeners attend to size information in domestic dog growls. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. J Acoust Soc Am, 123(5), 2903-2909. doi: 10.1121/1.2896962

Taylor, A. M., Reby, D., & McComb, K. (2009). Context-Related Variation in the Vocal Growling Behaviour of the Domestic Dog (Canis familiaris). Ethology, 115(10), 905-915. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01681.x

Taylor, A. M., Reby, D., & McComb, K. (2010). Size communication in domestic dog, Canis familiaris, growls. Animal Behaviour, 79(1), 205-210. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.10.030

Taylor, A. M., Reby, D., & McComb, K. (2010). Why Do Large Dogs Sound More Aggressive to Human Listeners: Acoustic Bases of Motivational Misattributions. Ethology, 116(12), 1155-1162. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01829.x

Taylor, R. C., Klein, B. A., Stein, J., & Ryan, M. J. (2008). Faux frogs: multimodal signalling and the value of robotics in animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 76(3), 1089-1097. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.031

Teichroeb, J. A., & Sicotte, P. (2010). The Function of Male Agonistic Displays in Ursine Colobus Monkeys (Colobus vellerosus): Male Competition, Female Mate Choice or Sexual Coercion? Ethology, 116(4), 366-380. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01752.x

Templeton, C. N., & Greene, E. (2007). Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 104(13), 5479-5482. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605183104

Templeton, C. N., Greene, E., & Davis, K. (2005). Allometry of alarm calls: Black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science, 308, 1934-1937.

Terrace, H. S. (1979). Nim. New York: Knopf.

Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence? Science, 206, 891-902.

Thompson, A. B., & Hare, J. F. (2010). Neighbourhood watch: multiple alarm callers communicate directional predator movement in Richardson’s ground squirrels, Spermophilus richardsonii. Animal Behaviour, 80(2), 269-275. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.028

Thompson, J. T., Bissell, A. N., & Martins, E. P. (2008). Inhibitory interactions between multimodal behavioural responses may influence the evolution of complex signals. Animal Behaviour, 76(1), 113-121. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.12.015

Thompson, N. S. (1982). A comparison of cawing in the European Carrion Crow (Corvus corone) and the American Common Crow (Corvus brachyrhynchos). Behaviour, 80, 106-117.

Thomsen, F., Franck, D., & Ford, J. (2002). On the communicative significance of whistles in wild killer whales ( Orcinus orca ). Naturwissenschaften, 89(9), 404-407. doi: 10.1007/s00114-002-0351-x

Thomsen, F., Franck, D., & Ford, J. K. (2002). On the communicative significance of whistles in wild killer whales (Orcinus orca). [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Naturwissenschaften, 89(9), 404-407. doi: 10.1007/s00114-002-0351-x

Tibbetts, E. A., & Dale, J. (2007). Individual recognition: it is good to be different. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 22(10), 529-537. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2007.09.001

Tibbetts, E. A., Sheehan, M. J., & Dale, J. (2008). A testable definition of individual recognition. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 23(7), 356. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.03.007

Tinghitella, R. M., Wang, J. M., & Zuk, M. (2009). Preexisting behavior renders a mutation adaptive: flexibility in male phonotaxis behavior and the loss of singing ability in the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Behavioral Ecology, 20(4), 722-728. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arp052

Topal, J., Gergely, G., Erdohegyi, A., Csibra, G., & Miklosi, A. (2009). Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves, and human infants. [Comparative Study

Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Science, 325(5945), 1269-1272. doi: 10.1126/science.1176960

Toro, J. M., Trobalon, J. B., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2005). Effects of backward speech and speaker variability in language discrimination by rats. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process, 31(1), 95-100. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.31.1.95

Townsend, S. W., Hollén, L. I., & Manser, M. B. (2010). Meerkat close calls encode group-specific signatures, but receivers fail to discriminate. Animal Behaviour, 80(1), 133-138. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.010

Trainer, J. M. (1989). Cultural evolution in song dialects of Yellow-rumped caciques in Panama. Ethology, 1989, 190-204.

Tricas, T. C., Kajiura, S. M., & Kosaki, R. K. (2006). Acoustic communication in territorial butterflyfish: test of the sound production hypothesis. [Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S.]. J Exp Biol, 209(Pt 24), 4994-5004. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02609

Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35-37.

Uetz, G. W., Roberts, J. A., & Taylor, P. W. (2009). Multimodal communication and mate choice in wolf spiders: female response to multimodal versus unimodal signals. Animal Behaviour, 78(2), 299-305. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.04.023

Van den Stock, J., Grezes, J., & de Gelder, B. (2008). Human and animal sounds influence recognition of body language. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Brain Res, 1242, 185-190. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.040

Van den Stock, J., Grèzes, J., & de Gelder, B. (2008). Human and animal sounds influence recognition of body language. Brain Research, 1242, 185-190. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.040

Vannoni, E., & McElligott, A. G. (2009). Fallow bucks get hoarse: vocal fatigue as a possible signal to conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 78(1), 3-10. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.03.015

Vas, J., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V. (2005). A friend or an enemy? Dogs’ reaction to an unfamiliar person showing behavioural cues of threat and friendliness at different times. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 94(1-2), 99-115. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.02.001

Verdolin, J. L., & Slobodchikoff, C. N. (2009). Resources, not Kinship, Determine Social Patterning in the Territorial Gunnison’s prairie Dog (Cynomys gunnisoni). Ethology, 115(1), 59-69. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01590.x

Vernes, S. C., Oliver, P. L., Spiteri, E., Lockstone, H. E., Puliyadi, R., Taylor, J. M., . . . Fisher, S. E. (2011). Foxp2 regulates gene networks implicated in neurite outgrowth in the developing brain. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. PLoS Genet, 7(7), e1002145. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002145

Vessel, E. A. (2010). Beauty and the beholder: Highly individual taste for abstract, but not real-world images. Journal of Vision, 10(2), 1-14. doi: 10.1167/10.2.18

Vessel, E. A., & Rubin, N. (2010). Beauty and the beholder: highly individual taste for abstract, but not real-world images. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. J Vis, 10(2), 18 11-14. doi: 10.1167/10.2.18

Vezenkov, S. R., & Danalev, D. L. (2009). From molecule to sexual behavior: the role of the neuropentapeptide proctolin in acoustic communication in the male grasshopper Chorthippus biguttulus. Eur J Pharmacol, 619(1-3), 57-60. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.08.010

Vigo, R., & Allen, C. (2009). How to reason without words: inference as categorization. Cogn Process, 10(1), 77-88. doi: 10.1007/s10339-008-0220-4

Vitale, A., Zanzoni, M., Queyras, A., & Chiarotti, F. (2003). Degree of social contact affects the emission of food calls in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Am J Primatol, 59(1), 21-28. doi: 10.1002/ajp.10060

Voigt-Heucke, S. L., Taborsky, M., & Dechmann, D. K. N. (2010). A dual function of echolocation: bats use echolocation calls to identify familiar and unfamiliar individuals. Animal Behaviour, 80(1), 59-67. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.025

Wager, B. R., & Breed, M. D. (2000). Does honey bee sting alarm pheromone give orientation information to defensive bees? Annals of the Entomological Society of America, 93(6), 1329-1332.

Waldman, B. (2004). Chemical communication in an archaic anuran amphibian. Behavioral Ecology, 15(1), 88-93. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arg071

Wallman, J. (1992). Aping Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Webb, D. M., & Zhang, J. (2005). FoxP2 in song-learning birds and vocal-learning mammals. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. J Hered, 96(3), 212-216. doi: 10.1093/jhered/esi025

Webster, H., McNutt, J. W., & McComb, K. (2010). Eavesdropping and Risk Assessment Between Lions, Spotted Hyenas and African Wild Dogs. Ethology, 116(3), 233-239. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01729.x

Wedell, N. (2007). Sperm competition and ejaculate evolution. Society of Reproduction and Fertility Supplement, 65, 115-135.

Węgrzyn, E., Leniowski, K., & Osiejuk, T. S. (2010). Whistle duration and consistency reflect philopatry and harem size in great reed warblers. Animal Behaviour, 79(6), 1363-1372. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.012

Welbergen, J. A., & Davies, N. B. (2008). Reed warblers discriminate cuckoos from sparrowhawks with graded alarm signals that attract mates and neighbours. Animal Behaviour, 76(3), 811-822. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.03.020

Wheeler, B. C. (2008). Selfish or altruistic? An analysis of alarm call function in wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus. Animal Behaviour, 76(5), 1465-1475. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.023

Wich, S. A., Delgado, R. A., & Lameira, A. R. (2010). Review of geographic variation in terrestrial mammalian acoustic signals: Human speech variation in a comparative perspective. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 8(4), 309-332. doi: 10.1556/jep.8.2010.4.2

Wickerthomas, C. (2007). Pheromonal communication involved in courtship behavior in Diptera. Journal of Insect Physiology, 53(11), 1089-1100. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2007.07.003

Wicker-Thomas, C. (2007). Pheromonal communication involved in courtship behavior in Diptera. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t

Review]. J Insect Physiol, 53(11), 1089-1100. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2007.07.003

Wicker-Thomas, C. (2007). Pheromonal communication involved in courtship behavior in Diptera. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Review]. J Insect Physiol, 53(11), 1089-1100. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2007.07.003

Wiebe, K. L., & Slagsvold, T. (2009). Mouth coloration in nestling birds: increasing detection or signalling quality? Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1413-1420. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.013

Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature, 308(5955), 181-183.

Williams, B. R., & Houtman, A. M. (2008). Song of Costa’s Hummingbird (Calypte Costae). The Auk, 125(3), 663-669. doi: 10.1525/auk.2008.07058

Wilson, D., & Evans, C. (2008). Mating success increases alarm-calling effort in male fowl, Gallus gallus. Animal Behaviour, 76(6), 2029-2035. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.08.021

Wilson, D. R., & Evans, C. S. (2008). Mating success increases alarm-calling effort in male fowl, Gallus gallus. Animal Behaviour, 76(6), 2029-2035. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.08.021

Wilson, D. R., & Evans, C. S. (2008). Mating success increases alarm-calling effort in male fowl, Gallus gallus. Animal Behaviour, 76(6), 2029-2035. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.08.021

Wilson, D. R., & Evans, C. S. (2012). Fowl communicate the size, speed and proximity of avian stimuli through graded structure in referential alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 83(2), 535-544. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.033

Wilson, D. R., & Mennill, D. J. (2010). Black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus, can use individually distinctive songs to discriminate among conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 79(6), 1267-1275. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.028

Wilson, E. O., & Holldobler, B. (2005). Eusociality: origin and consequences. [Review]. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 102(38), 13367-13371. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505858102

Wisenden, B. D., Binstock, C. L., Knoll, K. E., Linke, A. J., & Demuth, B. S. (2010). Risk-sensitive information gathering by cyprinids following release of chemical alarm cues. Animal Behaviour, 79(5), 1101-1107. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.004

Wright, J., Karasov, W. H., Kazem, A. J. N., Gonçalves, I. B., & McSwan, E. (2010). Begging and digestive responses to differences in long-term and short-term need in nestling pied flycatchers. Animal Behaviour, 80(3), 517-525. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.06.015

Wright, T. F. (1996). Regional dialects in the contact call of a parrot. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 263, 867-872.

Wright, T. F., Dahlin, C. R., & Salinas-Melgoza, A. (2008). Stability and change in vocal dialects of the yellow-naped amazon. Animal Behaviour, 76(3), 1017-1027. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.03.025

Wright, T. F., & Wilkinson, G. S. (2001). Population genetic structure and vocal dialects in an amazon parrot. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 268(1467), 609-616. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1403

Wyman, M. T., Mooring, M. S., McCowan, B., Penedo, M. C. T., & Hart, L. A. (2008). Amplitude of bison bellows reflects male quality, physical condition and motivation. Animal Behaviour, 76(5), 1625-1639. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.032

Wyman, M. T., Mooring, M. S., McCowan, B., Penedo, M. C. T., & Hart, L. A. (2008). Amplitude of bison bellows reflects male quality, physical condition and motivation. Animal Behaviour, 76(5), 1625-1639. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.032

Yovel, Y., Melcon, M. L., Franz, M. O., Denzinger, A., & Schnitzler, H.-U. (2009). The voice of bats: How greater mouse-eared bats recognize individuals based on their echolocation calls. PLoS Computational Biology, 5(6), e1000400. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000400.g001

Yovell. (2009). The Voice Of Individual Bats. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000400.g001

Zuberbuhler, K. (2000). Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 267, 713-718.

Zuberbühler, K. (2002). A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Animal Behaviour, 63(2), 293-299. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1914

Zuk, M., Rebar, D., & Scott, S. P. (2008). Courtship song is more variable than calling song in the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Animal Behaviour, 76(3), 1065-1071. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.018

Zuri, I., Dombrowski, K., & Halpern, M. (2005). Skin and gland but not urine odours elicit conspicuous investigation by female grey short-tailed opossums, Monodelphis domestica. Animal Behaviour, 69(3), 635-642. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.04.023