Ekman, P. (1992). Telling Lies: Clues To Deceit in the Marketplace, Politics, and Marriage. New York: Norton.
Elgier, A. M., Jakovcevic, A., Barrera, G., Mustaca, A. E., & Bentosela, M. (2009). Communication between domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans: dogs are good learners. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Review]. Behav Processes, 81(3), 402-408. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.03.017
Elie, J. E., Mariette, M. M., Soula, H. A., Griffith, S. C., Mathevon, N., & Vignal, C. (2010). Vocal communication at the nest between mates in wild zebra finches: a private vocal duet? Animal Behaviour, 80(4), 597-605. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.06.003
Ellis, J. M., Langen, T. A., & Berg, E. C. (2009). Signaling for food and sex? Begging by reproductive female white-throated magpie-jays. Anim Behav, 78(3), 615-623. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.024
Ellis, J. M. S., Langen, T. A., & Berg, E. C. (2009). Signalling for food and sex? Begging by reproductive female white-throated magpie-jays. Animal Behaviour, 78(3), 615-623. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.024
Enard, W., Przeworski, M., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S. L., Wiebe, V., Kitano, T., . . . Paabo, S. (2002). Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature, 418, 869-872.
Enggist-Dueblin, P. (2002). Cultural transmission of vocalizations in ravens, Corvus corax. Animal Behaviour, 64(6), 831-841. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.2016
Evans, C. S. (1997). Referential signals. In D. H. Owings, M. D. Beecher & N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives in Ethology:Communication (Vol. 12, pp. 99-143). New York: Plenum Press.
Evans, C. S., & Evans, L. (1999). Chicken food calls are functionally referential. Animal Behaviour, 58, 307-319.
Evans, C. S., & Evans, L. (2007). Representational signalling in birds. Biol Lett, 3(1), 8-11. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2006.0561
Evans, C. S., Evans, L., & Marler, P. (1993). On the meaning of alarm calls: functional reference in an avian vocal system. Animal Behaviour, 46, 23-38.
Evans, C. S., Macedonia, J. M., & Marler, P. (1993). Effects of apparent size and speed on the response of chickens, Gallus gallus, to computer-generated simulations of aerial predators. Animal Behaviour, 46, 1-11.
Evans, C. S., & Marler, P. (1994). Food calling and audience effects in male chickens, Gallus gallus: their relationship to food availability, courtship and social facilitation. Animal Behaviour, 47, 1159-1170.
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Behav Brain Sci, 32(5), 429-448; discussion 448-494. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999094X
Evans, T. A., Inta, R., Lai, J. C., Prueger, S., Foo, N. W., Fu, E. W., & Lenz, M. (2009). Termites eavesdrop to avoid competitors. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Proc Biol Sci, 276(1675), 4035-4041. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1147
Evans, T. A., Inta, R., Lai, J. C. S., Prueger, S., Foo, N. W., Fu, E. W., & Lenz, M. (2009). Termites eavesdrop to avoid competitors. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1675), 4035-4041. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1147
Evison, S. E. F., Hart, A. G., & Jackson, D. E. (2008). Minor workers have a major role in the maintenance of leafcutter ant pheromone trails. Animal Behaviour, 75(3), 963-969. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.013
Fairbanks, B., & Dobson, F. (2007). Mechanisms of the group-size effect on vigilance in Columbian ground squirrels: dilution versus detection. Animal Behaviour, 73(1), 115-123. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.07.002
Fairbanks, B., & Dobson, F. (2007). Mechanisms of the group-size effect on vigilance in Columbian ground squirrels: dilution versus detection. Animal Behaviour, 73(1), 115-123. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.07.002
Fairbanks, B., & Dobson, F. (2007). Mechanisms of the group-size effect on vigilance in Columbian ground squirrels: dilution versus detection. Animal Behaviour, 73(1), 115-123. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.07.002
Fallow, P. M., & Magrath, R. D. (2010). Eavesdropping on other species: mutual interspecific understanding of urgency information in avian alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 79(2), 411-417. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.018
Falls, J. B. (1985). Song matching in western meadowlarks. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 63, 2520-2524.
Falls, J. B., & D’Agincourt, L. G. (1981). A comparison of neighbor-stranger discrimination in eastern and western meadowlarks. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 59, 2380-2385.
Faragó, T., Pongrácz, P., Range, F., Virányi, Z., & Miklósi, Á. (2010). ‘The bone is mine’: affective and referential aspects of dog growls. Animal Behaviour, 79(4), 917-925. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.005
Faria, J. J., Dyer, J. R. G., Tosh, C. R., & Krause, J. (2010). Leadership and social information use in human crowds. Animal Behaviour, 79(4), 895-901. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.12.039
Farji-Brener, A. G., Amador-Vargas, S., Chinchilla, F., Escobar, S., Cabrera, S., Herrera, M. I., & Sandoval, C. (2010). Information transfer in head-on encounters between leaf-cutting ant workers: food, trail condition or orientation cues? Animal Behaviour, 79(2), 343-349. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.009
Feher, O., Wang, H., Saar, S., Mitra, P. P., & Tchernichovski, O. (2009). De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finch. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Nature, 459(7246), 564-568. doi: 10.1038/nature07994
Fehér, O., Wang, H., Saar, S., Mitra, P. P., & Tchernichovski, O. (2009). De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finch. Nature, 459(7246), 564-568. doi: 10.1038/nature07994
Feighny, J. A., Williamson, K. E., & Clarke, J. A. (2006). North American elk bugle vocalizations: Male and female bugle call structure and context. Journal of Mammalogy, 87(6), 1072-1077.
Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Manipulations of fundamental and formant frequencies influence the attractiveness of human male voices. Animal Behaviour, 69(3), 561-568. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.06.012
Feng, A. S., Narins, P. M., Xu, C. H., Lin, W. Y., Yu, Z. L., Qiu, Q., . . . Shen, J. X. (2006). Ultrasonic communication in frogs. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S.]. Nature, 440(7082), 333-336. doi: 10.1038/nature04416
Feng, A. S., Narins, P. M., Xu, C.-H., Lin, W.-Y., Yu, Z.-L., Qiu, Q., . . . Shen, J.-X. (2006). Ultrasonic communication in frogs. Nature, 440(7082), 333-336. doi: 10.1038/nature04416
Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., & McCowan, B. (2009). A Law of Word Meaning in Dolphin Whistle Types. Entropy, 11(4), 688-701. doi: 10.3390/e11040688
Fichtel, C. (2004). Reciprocal recognition of sifaka ( Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi) and redfronted lemur ( Eulemur fulvus rufus) alarm calls. Anim Cogn, 7(1), 45-52. doi: 10.1007/s10071-003-0180-0
Ficken, M. S., & Popp, J. (1996). A comparative analysis of passerine mobbing calls. The Auk, 113(2), 370-380.
Ficken, M. S., Rusch, K. M., Taylor, S. J., & Powers, D. R. (2000). Blue-throated hummingbird song: A pinnacle of nonoscine vocalizations. The Auk, 117, 120-128.
Ficken, M. S., Weise, C. M., & Reinartz, J. A. (1987). A complex vocalization of the black-capped chickadee. II. Repertoires, dominance and dialects. The Condor, 89, 500-509.
Fischer, J., Hammerschmidt, K., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2002). Acoustic features of male baboon loud calls: Influences of context, age, and individuality. J Acoust Soc Am, 111(3), 1465. doi: 10.1121/1.1433807
Fischer, J., Kitchen, D. M., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2004). Baboon loud calls advertise male quality: acoustic features and their relation to rank, age, and exhaustion. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56(2), 140-148. doi: 10.1007/s00265-003-0739-4
Fischer, J., Metz, M., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2001). Baboon responses to graded bark variants. Animal Behaviour, 61(5), 925-931. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1687
Fisher, S. E., & Scharff, C. (2009). FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Review]. Trends Genet, 25(4), 166-177. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2009.03.002
Fitch, W. T. (2006). The biology and evolution of music: a comparative perspective. [Comparative Study Review]. Cognition, 100(1), 173-215. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.009
Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., & Chomsky, N. (2005). The evolution of the language faculty: clarifications and implications. Cognition, 97(2), 179-210; discussion 211-125. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.005
Font, E., & Carazo, P. (2010). Animals in translation: why there is meaning (but probably no message) in animal communication. Animal Behaviour, 80(2), e1-e6. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.05.015
Foote, J. R., Fitzsimmons, L. P., Mennill, D. J., & Ratcliffe, L. M. (2008). Male chickadees match neighbors interactively at dawn: support for the social dynamics hypothesis. Behavioral Ecology, 19(6), 1192-1199. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arn087
Fouts, R., & Mills, S. T. (1997). Next of Kin: My Conversations with Chimpanzees. New York: William Morrow.
Frederiksen, J. K., & Slobodchikoff, C. N. (2007). Referential specificity in the alarm calls of the black-tailed prairie dog. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 19, 87-99.
Frederiksen, J. K., & Slobodchikoff, C. N. (2007). Referential specificity in the alarm calls of the black-tailed prairie dogs. Ethology, Ecology & Evolution, 19, 87-99.
Freeberg, T. M. (2008). Complexity in Thechick-a-Deecall of Carolina Chickadees (Poecile Carolinensis): Associations of Context and Signaler Behavior to Call Structure. The Auk, 125(4), 896-907. doi: 10.1525/auk.2008.07025
Fripp, D., & Tyack, P. (2008). Postpartum whistle production in bottlenose dolphins. Marine Mammal Science, 24(3), 479-502.
Frisch, K. v. (1967). The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees (L. E. Chadwick, Trans.). Cambridge MA: Belknap Press.
Frisch, K. v. (1974). Decoding The Language Of The Bee. Science, 185(4152), 663-668.
Frommolt, K.-H., Goltsman, M. E., & Macdonald, D. W. (2003). Barking foxes, Alopex lagopus: field experiments in individual recognition in a territorial mammal. Animal Behaviour, 65(3), 509-518. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2064
Fukuzawa, M., Mills, D. S., & Cooper, J. J. (2005). More than just a word: non-semantic command variables affect obedience in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 91(1-2), 129-141. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.08.025
Furrer, R. D., & Manser, M. B. (2009). Banded mongoose recruitment calls convey information about risk and not stimulus type. Animal Behaviour, 78(1), 195-201. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.002
Furrer, R. D., & Manser, M. B. (2009). The evolution of urgency-based and functionally referential alarm calls in ground-dwelling species. American Naturalist, 173(3), 400-410.
Galusha, J. G., & Stout, J. F. (1977). Aggressive communication by Larus glaucescens. Part IV. Experiments on visual communication. Behaviour, 62, 222-235.
Gardner, K., Seeley, T., & Calderone, N. (2008). Do honeybees have two discrete dances to advertise food sources? Animal Behaviour, 75(4), 1291-1300. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.032
Gardner, K. E., Seeley, T. D., & Calderone, N. W. (2008). Do honeybees have two discrete dances to advertise food sources? Animal Behaviour, 75(4), 1291-1300. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.032
Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T., & Van Cantfort, T. E. (1989). Teaching Sign Language to Chimpanzees. Albany NY: State University of New York Press.
Garland, E. C., Goldizen, A. W., Rekdahl, M. L., Constantine, R., Garrigue, C., Hauser, N. D., . . . Noad, M. J. (2011). Dynamic horizontal cultural transmission of humpback whale song at the ocean basin scale. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S.]. Curr Biol, 21(8), 687-691. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.019
Gentner, T. Q., Fenn, K. M., Margoliash, D., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2006). Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Nature, 440(7088), 1204-1207. doi: 10.1038/nature04675
Gerald, M. S., Waitt, C., & Little, A. C. (2009). Pregnancy coloration in macaques may act as a warning signal to reduce antagonism by conspecifics. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Behav Processes, 80(1), 7-11. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.08.001
Gibson, J., & Uetz, G. (2008). Seismic communication and mate choice in wolf spiders: components of male seismic signals and mating success. Animal Behaviour, 75(4), 1253-1262. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.026
Gibson, J. S., & Uetz, G. W. (2008). Seismic communication and mate choice in wolf spiders: components of male seismic signals and mating success. Animal Behaviour, 75(4), 1253-1262. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.026
Gil, M., & De Marco, R. J. (2010). Decoding information in the honeybee dance: revisiting the tactile hypothesis. Animal Behaviour, 80(5), 887-894. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.08.012
Gil-da-Costa, R., Palleroni, A., Hauser, M. D., Touchton, J., & Kelley, J. P. (2003). Rapid acquisition of an alarm response by a neotropical primate to a newly introduced avian predator. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Proc Biol Sci, 270(1515), 605-610. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2281
Gill, S. A., & Sealy, S. G. (2004). Functional reference in an alarm signal given during nest defence: seet calls of yellow warblers denote brood-parasitic brown-headed cowbirds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56(1), 71-80. doi: 10.1007/s00265-003-0736-7
Giret, N., Peron, F., Nagle, L., Kreutzer, M., & Bovet, D. (2009). Spontaneous categorization of vocal imitations in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Behav Processes, 82(3), 244-248. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.07.001
Giret, N., Péron, F., Nagle, L., Kreutzer, M., & Bovet, D. (2009). Spontaneous categorization of vocal imitations in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Behavioural Processes, 82(3), 244-248. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.07.001
Gittleman, J. L., & Thompson, S. D. (1988). Energy allocation in mammalian reproduction. American Zoologist, 28, 863-875.
Gluckman, T.-L., & Cardoso, G. C. (2009). A method to quantify the regularity of barred plumage patterns. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63(12), 1837-1844. doi: 10.1007/s00265-009-0823-5
González, C., & Ornelas, J. F. (2009). Song Variation and Persistence of Song Neighborhoods in a Lekking Hummingbird. The Condor, 111(4), 633-640. doi: 10.1525/cond.2009.090029
Gouzoules, H., & Gouzoules, S. (1989). Design features and developmental modification of pigtail macaque, Macaca nemestrina, agonistic screams. Animal Behaviour, 37, 383-401.
Gouzoules, H., & Gouzoules, S. (2000). Agonistic screams differ among four species of macaques: the significance of motivation-structural rules. Anim Behav, 59(3), 501-512. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1318
Gouzoules, S., Gouzoules, H., & Marler, P. (1984). Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) screams: Representational signalling in the recruitment of agonistic aid. Animal Behaviour, 32, 182-193.
Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in Pictures, and Other Reports from My Life with Autism. New York: Vintage Books.
Greene, E., & Meagher, T. (1998). Red squirrels, Tamiasciurus hudsonicus, produce predator-class specific alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 55, 511-518.
Gregory, S. W., Jr., & Gallagher, T. J. (2002). Spectral analysis of candidates’ nonverbal vocal communication: Predicting U. S. presidential election outcomes. Social Psychology Quarterly, 65(3), 298-308.
Griesser, M., & Ekman, J. (2004). Nepotistic alarm calling in the Siberian jay, Perisoreus infaustus. Animal Behaviour, 67(5), 933-939. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.09.005
Grinnell, J., & McComb, K. (2001). Roaring and social communication in African lions: the limitations imposed by listeners. Animal Behaviour, 62(1), 93-98. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1735
Gruter, C., & Farina, W. M. (2009). The honeybee waggle dance: can we follow the steps? [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Trends Ecol Evol, 24(5), 242-247. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.12.007
Grüter, C., & Farina, W. M. (2009). The honeybee waggle dance: can we follow the steps? Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 24(5), 242-247. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.12.007
Grüter, C., & Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2011). Honeybee foragers increase the use of waggle dance information when private information becomes unrewarding. Animal Behaviour, 81(5), 949-954. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.014
Gueguen, N., Jacob, C., & Lamy, L. (2010). “Love is in the air”: Effects of songs with romantic lyrics on compliance with a courtship request. Psychology of Music, 38(3), 303-307.
Guinee, L. N., & Payne, K. B. (1988). Rhyme-like repetitions in songs of humpback whales. Ethology, 79, 295-306.
Haesler, S., Wada, K., Nshdejan, A., Morrisey, E. E., Lints, T., Jarvis, E. D., & Scharff, C. (2004). FoxP2 expression in avian vocal learners and non-learners. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 3164-3175.
Hailman, J. P., & Ficken, M. S. (1986). Combinatorial animal communication with computable sythax: Chick-a-dee calling qualifies as “language” by structural linguistics. Animal Behaviour, 34, 1899-1901.
Hailman, J. P., Ficken, M. S., & Ficken, R. W. (1985). The “chick-a-dee” calls of Parus atricapillus: A recombinant system of animal communication compared with written English. Semiotica, 56, 191-224.
Hailman, J. P., & Griswold, C. K. (1996). Syntax of black-capped chickadee (Parus atricapillus) gargles sorts many types into few groups: Implications for geographic variation, dialect drift, and vocal learning. Bird Behavior, 11, 39-57.
Halfwerk, W., & Slabbekoorn, H. (2009). A behavioural mechanism explaining noise-dependent frequency use in urban birdsong. Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1301-1307. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.015
Hardus, M. E., Lameira, A. R., Van Schaik, C. P., & Wich, S. A. (2009). Tool use in wild orang-utans modifies sound production: A functionally deceptive innovation? [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Proc Biol Sci, 276(1673), 3689-3694. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1027
Hare, J., & Atkins, B. (2001). The squirrel that cried wolf: reliability detection by juvenile Richardson’s ground squirrels ( Spermophilus richardsonii ). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 51(1), 108-112. doi: 10.1007/s002650100414
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569-1579.
Hauser, M. D., Evans, C. S., & Marler, P. (1993). The role of articulation in the production of rhesus monkey, Macaca mulatta, vocalizations. Animal Behaviour, 45, 423-433.
Hauser, M. D., & Marler, P. (1993). Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): I. Socioecological factors. Behavioral Ecology, 4(3), 194-205.
Hauser, M. D., & Marler, P. (1993). Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): II. Costs and benefits of call production and suppression. Behavioral Ecology, 4(3), 206-212.
Herbinger, I., Papworth, S., Boesch, C., & Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Vocal, gestural and locomotor responses of wild chimpanzees to familiar and unfamiliar intruders: a playback study. Animal Behaviour, 78(6), 1389-1396. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.010
Herman, L. M. (1986). Cognition and language competencies of bottlenosed dolphins. In R. J. Shusterman, J. A. Thomas & F. G. Woods (Eds.), Dolphin Cognition and Behavior: A Comparative Approach (pp. 221-252). Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Herman, L. M., Richards, D. G., & Wolz, J. P. (1984). Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins. Cognition, 16, 129-219.
Hewes, G. W. (1977). Language origin theories. In D. M. Rumbaugh (Ed.), Language Learning By A Chimpanzee: The LANA Project (pp. 3-53). New York: Academic Press.
Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2011). The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Anim Cogn, 14(5), 745-767. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0409-2
Hockett, C. F. (1960). Logical considerations in the study of animal communication. In W. E. Lanyon & W. N. Tavolga (Eds.), Animal Sounds and Communication (pp. 392-430). Washington D. C.: American Institute of Biological Sciences.
Hof, D., & Hazlett, N. (2010). Low-amplitude song predicts attack in a North American wood warbler. Animal Behaviour, 80(5), 821-828. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.07.017
Holldobler, B. (1999). Multimodal signals in ant communication. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 184, 129-141.
Holldobler, B., Braun, U., Gronenberg, W., Kirchner, W., & Peeters, C. (1994). Trail communication in the ant Megaponera foetens (Fabr.) (Formicidae, Ponerinae). Journal of Insect Physiology, 40, 585-593.
Holldobler, B., & Wilson, E. O. (2011). The Leafcutter Ants. New York: Norton.
Hollén, L. I., & Radford, A. N. (2009). The development of alarm call behaviour in mammals and birds. Animal Behaviour, 78(4), 791-800. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.021
Hollister-Smith, J. A., Alberts, S. C., & Rasmussen, L. E. L. (2008). Do male African elephants, Loxodonta africana, signal musth via urine dribbling? Animal Behaviour, 76(6), 1829-1841. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.033
Holy, T. E., & Guo, Z. (2005). Ultrasonic songs of male mice. PLos Biology, 3(12), e386. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030386.g001
Hopp, S. L., Jablonski, P., & Brown, J. L. (2001). Recognition of group membership by voice in Mexican jays, Aphelocoma ultramarina. Animal Behaviour, 62(2), 297-303. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1745
Hostetter, A. B., Cantero, M., & Hopkins, W. D. (2001). Differential use of vocal and gestural communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in response to attentional status of a human (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(4), 337-343. doi: 10.10377/0735-7036.115.4.337
How, M. J., Hemmi, J. M., Zeil, J., & Peters, R. (2008). Claw waving display changes with receiver distance in fiddler crabs, Uca perplexa. Animal Behaviour, 75(3), 1015-1022. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.004
Jackendoff, R., & Lerdahl, F. (2006). The capacity for music: what is it, and what’s special about it? [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Review]. Cognition, 100(1), 33-72. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.005
Janicke, T., Hahn, S., Ritz, M. S., & Peter, H.-U. (2008). Vocal performance reflects individual quality in a nonpasserine. Animal Behaviour, 75(1), 91-98. doi: 0.1016/j.anbehav.2007.04.007
Janik, V. M., Sayigh, L. S., & Wells, R. S. (2006). Signature whistle shape conveys identity information to bottlenose dolphins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 103(21), 8293-8297.
Jensen, K. K., Larsen, O. N., & Attenborough, K. (2008). Measurements and predictions of hooded crow (Corvus corone cornix) call propagation over open field habitats. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. J Acoust Soc Am, 123(1), 507. doi: 10.1121/1.2817363
Jensvold, M. L. A., & Gardner, R. A. (2000). Interactive use of sign language by cross-fostered chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114(4), 335-346. doi: 10.1037//0735-7036.114.4.335
Jones, B. C., Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., & Vukovic, J. (2010). A domain-specific opposite-sex bias in human preferences for manipulated voice pitch. Animal Behaviour, 79(1), 57-62. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.10.003
Jones, K. J., & Hill, W. L. (2001). Auditory perception of hawks and owls for passerine alarm calls. Ethology, 107, 717-726.
Jordan, K. E., Brannon, E. M., Logothetis, N. K., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2005). Monkeys match the number of voices they hear to the number of faces they see. [Comparative Study Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, Non-P.H.S.Research Support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.]. Curr Biol, 15(11), 1034-1038. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.04.056
Juslin, P. N., & Vastfjall, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: the need to consider underlying mechanisms. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Behav Brain Sci, 31(5), 559-575; discussion 575-621. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08005293